Skip to main content

Responsive Design is not just Two-Phase Sampling

I recently gave, along with Brady West, a short course on paradata and responsive design. We had a series of slides on what is "responsive design." I had a slide with a title similar to that of this post. I think it was "Responsive Design is not equal to Two-Phase Sampling."

I sometimes have a discussion with people about using "responsive design" on their survey, but I get the sense that what they really want to know about is two-phase sampling for nonresponse.

In fact, two-phase sampling, to be efficient, should have different cost structures across the phases. But the requirements for a responsive design are higher than that. Groves and Heeringa also argued that the phases should have 'complementary' design features. That is, each phase should be attractive to different kinds of sampled people. The hope is that nonresponse biases of prior phases are cancelled out by the biases of subsequent phases.

Further, responsive designs can exist without two-phase sampling. Design features can be complementary, have similar costs, and not necessarily be offered to subsets of nonrespondents from the prior phase.

Two-phase sampling for nonresponse was an important innovation. But I'd argue that Groves and Heeringa also came up with an important innovation which is actually different and new.

Comments

  1. Regarding the first reason you give about why responsive design is not two-phase sampling: can't each phase in a two-phase (or multi-phase) sampling also be attractive to different kinds of sampled people? The way I see, two-phase sampling is just the method, but you can use different, complementary design features in each phase, so that you attract different types of respondents. In that sense to me, responsive design is not different from two-phase sampling, it's just that the former used the latter to implement it.

    Can you give an example of a responsive design that does not use two-phase sampling, as you suggest in the forth paragraph?

    ReplyDelete
  2. With regard to the first reason, my point is that the sampling isn't a design feature. Something else has to change. The original Hansen and Hurwitz article changed the mode. I find that non-samplers tend to think of two-phase sampling as somehow magic. Almost like a way to cheat on the response rate. So, emphasizing the other design change forces them to think about the possibilities.

    On your second question, if I had a design feature that didn't cost more, then I could at least consider switching the design without sampling. For example, if I want to offer a shortened survey to nonresponders, I don't necessarily have to subsample in order to do this. The sampling is usually related to costs... or rare resources. For instance, you might subsample in order to give the remaining sample to a subset of the "best" interviewers, however, that is defined.

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

Tailoring vs. Targeting

One of the chapters in a recent book on surveying hard-to-reach populations looks at "targeting and tailoring" survey designs. The chapter references this paper on the use of the terms among those who design health communication. I thought the article was an interesting one. They start by saying that "one way to classify message strategies like tailoring is by the level of specificity with which characteristics of the target audience are reflected in the the communication." That made sense. There is likely a continuum of specificity ranging from complete non-differentiation across units to nearly individualized. But then the authors break that continuum and try to define a "fundamental" difference between tailoring and targeting. They say targeting is for some subgroup while tailoring is to the characteristics of the individual. That sounds good, but at least for surveys, I'm not sure the distinction holds. In survey design, what would constitute

What is Data Quality, and How to Enhance it in Research

  We often talk about “data quality” or “data integrity” when we are discussing the collection or analysis of one type of data or another. Yet, the definition of these terms might be unclear, or they may vary across different contexts. In any event, the terms are somewhat abstract -- which can make it difficult, in practice, to improve. That is, we need to know what we are describing with those terms, before we can improve them. Over the last two years, we have been developing a course on   Total Data Quality , soon to be available on Coursera. We start from an error classification scheme adopted by survey methodology many years ago. Known as the “Total Survey Error” perspective, it focuses on the classification of errors into measurement and representation dimensions. One goal of our course is to expand this classification scheme from survey data to other types of data. The figure shows the classification scheme as we have modified it to include both survey data and organic forms of d

An Experimental Adaptive Contact Strategy

I'm running an experiment on contact methods in a telephone survey. I'm going to present the results of the experiment at the FCSM conference in November. Here's the basic idea. Multi-level models are fit daily with the household being a grouping factor. The models provide household-specific estimates of the probability of contact for each of four call windows. The predictor variables in this model are the geographic context variables available for an RDD sample. Let $\mathbf{X_{ij}}$ denote a $k_j \times 1$ vector of demographic variables for the $i^{th}$ person and $j^{th}$ call. The data records are calls. There may be zero, one, or multiple calls to household in each window. The outcome variable is an indicator for whether contact was achieved on the call. This contact indicator is denoted $R_{ijl}$ for the $i^{th}$ person on the $j^{th}$ call to the $l^{th}$ window. Then for each of the four call windows denoted $l$, a separate model is fit where each household is assu