Skip to main content

How can we estimate "e"?

AAPOR defines response rates that include an adjustment factor for cases that have unknown eligibility at the end of the survey. They call the factor "e". Typically, people use the eligibility rate from the part of the sample where this variable (eligible=yes/no) is observed. This estimate is sometimes called the CASRO estimate of e.

But in a telephone survey, this estimate of "e" is likely to be biased upwards for the unknown part of the sample. Many of the cases that are never contacted are not households. They are simply numbers that will ring when dialed, but are not assigned to a household. These cases are never involved in estimates of "e".

A paper in POQ (Brick and Montaquila, 2002) described an alternative method of estimating e. They use a survival model. This lowers estimates of e relative to the CASRO method. But it's still upwardly biased since many of the noncontacts could never be contacted.

I like the survival method since it's closer to reality. But, for other reasons, I started imputing eligibility. I like this approach as it develops a nice range of estimates. And it allows great flexibility. It's very easy to include covariates in the model. It's not as easy to include covariates in the survival model.

Comments

  1. I like this, James, and may even try it too. I am wary of how it can be used, as simple model misspecification can lead to more bias in "e" that through the biased estimation using those for whom eligibility is established. Maybe I am wrong, but I have not thought about how to specify such a model enough. I guess I suspect that using number of calls, for example, may lead to a downward bias in "e".

    ReplyDelete
  2. I would argue that these sorts of models always overestimate "e". The problem is the set of numbers that will ring, but are not in fact assigned to households. Since none of these are ever resolved (even as nonsample), we apply the e from the other part of the sample to these cases.

    I think using the number of calls is similar to Brick and Montquilla's survival model, but you can also add covariates.

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

Assessment of Maching Learning Classifiers

I heard another interesting episode of the Data Skeptic podcast . They were discussing how a classifier could be assessed (episode 121). Many machine learning models are so complex that a human being can't really interpret the meaning of the model. This can lead to problems. They gave an example of a problem where they had a bunch of posts from two discussion boards. One was atheist and the other board was composed of Christians. They tried to classify each post as being from one or the other board. There was one poster who posted heavily on the Christian board. His name was Keith. Sadly, the model learned that if the person who was posting was named Keith, then they were Christian. The problem is that this isn't very useful for prediction. It's an artifact of the input data. Even cross-validation would eliminate this problem. A human being can see the issue, but a model can't. In any event, the proposed solution was to build interpretable models in local areas of t...

Tailoring vs. Targeting

One of the chapters in a recent book on surveying hard-to-reach populations looks at "targeting and tailoring" survey designs. The chapter references this paper on the use of the terms among those who design health communication. I thought the article was an interesting one. They start by saying that "one way to classify message strategies like tailoring is by the level of specificity with which characteristics of the target audience are reflected in the the communication." That made sense. There is likely a continuum of specificity ranging from complete non-differentiation across units to nearly individualized. But then the authors break that continuum and try to define a "fundamental" difference between tailoring and targeting. They say targeting is for some subgroup while tailoring is to the characteristics of the individual. That sounds good, but at least for surveys, I'm not sure the distinction holds. In survey design, what would constitute ...

What is Data Quality, and How to Enhance it in Research

  We often talk about “data quality” or “data integrity” when we are discussing the collection or analysis of one type of data or another. Yet, the definition of these terms might be unclear, or they may vary across different contexts. In any event, the terms are somewhat abstract -- which can make it difficult, in practice, to improve. That is, we need to know what we are describing with those terms, before we can improve them. Over the last two years, we have been developing a course on   Total Data Quality , soon to be available on Coursera. We start from an error classification scheme adopted by survey methodology many years ago. Known as the “Total Survey Error” perspective, it focuses on the classification of errors into measurement and representation dimensions. One goal of our course is to expand this classification scheme from survey data to other types of data. The figure shows the classification scheme as we have modified it to include both survey data and organic f...